Archive

Posts Tagged ‘precedence’

Parsing R code: Freedom of expression is not always a good idea

February 29, 2012 9 comments

With my growing interest in R it was inevitable that I would end up writing a parser for it. The fact that the language is relatively small (the add-on packages do the serious work) hastened the event because it did not look like much work; famous last words. I knew about R’s design and implementation being strongly influenced by the world view of functional programming and this should have set alarm bells ringing; this community have a history of willfully ignoring some of the undesirable consequences of their penchant of taking simple ideas and over generalizing them (i.e., I should have expected hidden complications).

While the official R language definition only contains a tiny fraction of the information needed to create a full implementation I decided to use it rather than ‘cheat’ and look at the R project implementation sources. I took as my oracle of correctness the source code of the substantial amount of R in its 3,000+ package library. This approach would help me uncover some of the incorrect preconceived ideas I have about how R source fits together.

I started with a C lexer and chopped and changed (it is difficult to do decent error recovery in automatically generated lexers and I prefer to avoid them). A few surprises cropped up ** is supported as an undocumented form of ^ and by default ]] must be treated as two tokens (e.g., two ] in a[b[c]] but one ]] in d[[e]], an exception to the very commonly used maximal munch rule).

The R grammar is all about expressions with some statement bits and pieces thrown in. R operator precedence follows that of Fortran, except the precedence of unary plus/minus has been increased to be above multiply/divide (instead of below). Easy peasy, cut and paste an existing expression grammar and done by tea time :-). Several tea times later I have a grammar that parses all of the R packages (apart from 80+ real syntax errors contained therein and a hand full of kinky operator combinations I’m not willing to contort the grammar to support). So what happened?

Two factors accounts for most of the difference between my estimate of the work required and the actual work required:

  • my belief that a well written grammar has no ambiguities (while zero is a common goal for many projects a handful might be acceptable if the building is on fire and I have to leave). A major advantage of automatic generation of parser tables from a grammar specification is being warned about ambiguities in the grammar (either shift/reduce or reduce/reduce conflicts). At an early stage I was pulling my hair out over having 59 conflicts and decided to relent and look at the R project source and was amazed to find their grammar has 81 ambiguities!

    I have managed to get the number of ambiguities down to the mid-30s, not good at all but it will have to do for the time being.

  • some of my preconceptions about of how R syntax worked were seriously wrong. In some cases I spotted my mistake quickly because I recognized the behavior from other languages I know, other misconceptions took a lot longer to understand and handle because I did not believe anybody would design expression evaluation to work that way.

The root cause of the difference can more concretely be traced to the approach to specifying language syntax. The R project grammar is written using the form commonly seen in functional language implementations and introductory compiler books. This form has the advantage of being very short and apparently simple; the following is a cut down example in a form of BNF used by many parser generators:

expr :   expr  op  expr  |
         IDENTIFIER      ;
 
op :  &'  |  '=='  |  '>'  |  '+'  |  '-'  |  '*'  |  '/'  | '^' ;

This specifies a sequence of IDENTIFIERs separated by binary operators and is ambiguous when the expression contains more than two operators, e.g., a + b * c can be parsed in more than one way. Parser generators such as Yacc will complain and flag any ambiguity and pick one of the possibilities as the default behavior for handling a given ambiguity; developers can specify additional grammar information in the file read by Yacc to guide its behavior when deciding how to resolve specific ambiguities. For instance, the relative precedence of operators can be specified and this information would be used by Yacc to decide that the ambiguous expression a + b * c should be parsed as-if it had been written like a + (b * c) rather than like (a + b) * c. The R project grammar is short, highly ambiguous and relies on the information contained in the explicitly specified relative operator precedence and associativity directives to resolve the ambiguities.

An alternative method of specifying the grammar is to have a separate list of grammar rules for each level of precedence (I always use this approach). In this approach there is no ambiguity, the precedence and associativity are implicitly specified by how the grammar is written. Obviously this approach creates much longer grammars, there will be at least two rules for every precedence level (19 in R, many with multiple operators). The following is a cut down example containing just multiple, divide, add and subtract:

...
multiplicative_expression:
             cast_expression                               |
             multiplicative_expression '*' cast_expression |
             multiplicative_expression '/' cast_expression ;
 
additive_expression:
             multiplicative_expression                         |
             additive_expression '+' multiplicative_expression |
             additive_expression '-' multiplicative_expression ;
...

The advantages of this approach are that, because there are no ambiguities, the developer can see exactly how the grammar behaves and if an ambiguity is accidentally introduced when editing the source it should be noticed when the parser generator reports a problem where previously there were none (rather than the new ambiguity being hidden in the barrage of existing ones that are ignored because they are so numerous).

My first big misconception about R syntax was to think that R had statements, it doesn’t. What other languages would treat as statements R always treats as expressions. The if, for and while constructs have values (e.g., 2*(if (x == y) 2 else 4)). No problem, I used Algol 68 as an undergraduate, which supports this kind of usage. I assumed that when an if appeared as an operand in an expression it would have to be bracketed using () or {} to avoid creating a substantial number of parsing ambiguities; WRONG. No brackets need be specified, the R expression if (x == y) 2 else 4+1 is ambiguous (it could be treated as-if it had been written if (x == y) 2 else (4+1) or (if (x == y) 2 else 4)+1) and the R project grammar relies on its precedence specification to resolve the conflict (in favor of the first possibility listed above).

My next big surprise came from the handling of unary operators. Most modern languages give all unary operators the same precedence, generally higher than any binary operator. Following Fortran the R unary operators have a variety of different precedence levels; however R did not adopt the restrictions Fortran places on where unary operators can occur.

I assumed that R had adopted the restrictions used by other languages containing unary operators at different precedence levels, e.g., not allowing a unary operator token to follow a binary operator token (i.e., there has to be an intervening opening parenthesis); WRONG. R allows me to write 1 == !1 < 0, while Fortran (and Ada, etc) require that a parenthesis be inserted between the operands == ! (hopefully resulting in the intent being clearer).

I had to think a bit to come up with an explicit set of grammar rules to handle R unary operator's freedom of expression (without creating any ambiguities).

Stepping back from the details. My view is that programming language syntax should be designed to reduce the number of mistakes developers make. Requiring that brackets appear in certain contexts helps prevent mistakes by the original author and subsequent readers of the code.

Claims that R (or any other language) syntax is 'natural' is clearly spurious and really no more than a statement of preference by the speaker. Our DNA has not yet been found to equip us to handle one programming language better than another.

Over the coming months I hope to have the time to analyse R source looking for faults that might not have occurred had brackets been used. Also how much code might be broken if R started to require brackets in certain contexts?

An example of the difference that brackets can make is provided by the handling of the unary ! operator in R and C/C++/Java/etc. Take the expression !x > y, which R parses as-if written !(x > y) and C/C++/Java/etc as if written (!x) > y. I would not claim that either is better than the other from the point of view of developers getting the behavior right, I know that some C programmers get it wrong and I suspect that some R programmers do too.

By increasing the precedence of unary plus/minus the R designers ensured that 8/-2/2 was parsed like (8/-2)/2 rather than 8/(-2/2).

Relative spacing of operands affects perception of operator precedence

January 22, 2012 1 comment

What I found most intriguing about Google Code Search (shutdown Nov 2011) was how quickly searches involving regular expressions returned matches. A few days ago Russ Cox, the implementor of Code Search not only explained how it worked but also released the source and some precompiled binaries. Google’s database of source code did not include the source of R, so I decided to install CodeSearch on my local machine and run some of my previous searches against the latest (v2.14.1) R source.

In 2007 I ran an experiment that showed developers made use of variable names when making binary operator precedence decisions. At about the same time two cognitive psychologists, David Landy and Robert Goldstone, were investigating the impact of spacing on operator precedence decisions (they found that readers showed a tendency to pair together the operands that were visibly closer to each other, e.g., a with b in a+b * c rather than b with c).

As somebody very interested in finding faults in code the psychologists research findings on spacing immediately suggested to me the possibility that ‘incorrectly’ spaced expressions were a sign of failure to write code that had the intended behavior. Feeding some rather complicated regular expressions into Google’s CodeSearch threw up a number of ‘incorrectly’ spaced expressions. However, this finding went no further than an interesting email exchange with Landy and Goldstone.

Time to find out whether there are any ‘incorrectly’ spaced expressions in the R source. cindex (the tool that builds the database used by csearch) took 3 seconds on a not very fast machine to process all of the R source (56M byte) and build the search database (10M byte; the Linux database is a factor of 5.5 smaller than the sources).

The search:

csearch "w(+|-)w +(*|/) +w"

returned a few interesting matches:

...
modules/internet/nanohttp.c:       used += tv_save.tv_sec + 1e-6 * tv_save.tv_usec;
modules/lapack/dlapack0.f:     $          ( T*( ONE+SQRT( ONE+S / T ) ) ) )
modules/lapack/dlapack2.f:               S = Z( 3 )*( Z( 2 ) / ( T*( ONE+SQRT( ONE+S / T ) ) ) )
modules/lapack/dlapack4.f:     $          ( T*( ONE+SQRT( ONE+S / T ) ) ) )

There were around 15 matches of code like 1e-6 * var (because the pattern w is for alphanumeric sequences and that is not a superset of the syntax of floating-point literals).

The subexpression ONE+S / T is just the sort of thing I was looking for. The three instances all involved code that processed tridiagonal matrices in various special cases. Google search combined with my knowledge of numerical analysis was not up to the task of figuring out whether the intended usage was (ONE+S)/T or ONE+(S/T).

Searches based on various other combination of operator pairs failed to match anything that looked suspicious.

There was an order of magnitude performance difference for csearch vs. grep -R -e (real 0m0.167s vs. real 0m2.208s). A very worthwhile improvement when searching much larger code bases with more complicated patterns.

Information content of expressions

December 11, 2009 No comments

Software developers read source code to obtain information. How might the information content of source code be quantified?

Both of the following functions assign the same value to x and if that is the only information a reader of that code is interested in, then the information content of both assignment statements could be said to be the same.

int foo(void)
{
x = 5;
...
}
 
int bar(void)
{
x = 2 + 3;
...

A reader seeking deeper understanding of the above code would ask why the value 5 is built from two values in bar. One reason might be that the author of the function wanted to explicitly call out background information about how the value 5 was derived (this is often done using symbolic names, but the use of literals themselves is sometimes encountered). Perhaps the author of foo did not see the need to expose this information or perhaps the shared value is purely coincidental.

If the two representations denote the same quantity doesn’t the second have a greater information content for a reader seeking deeper understanding?

In the following example:

... x + y & z ...
 
...
 
... num_red + num_white & lower_bits ...

an experienced developer with a knowledge of English is likely to interpret the expression as adding the number of occurrences of two quantities and using bit-wise AND to extract the lower bits. For some readers the second expression has a higher information content. Would use of the names number_of_red further increase the information content?

In the following example the first expression has not added any information that was not already present in the first expression above (except perhaps that the author was not certain of the precedence or perhaps did not expect subsequent readers to be certain).

... ( x + y ) & z ...
 
...
 
... x + ( y & z ) ...

The second expression uses parenthesis to achieve an operand/operator binding that is different from the default. Has this changed the information content of the expression?

There is experimental evidence that developers extract information from the names of variables to help them make decisions about operator precedence. To me the name all_32_bits_one suggests a sequence of bits and I would expect such a representation to be associated with the bit-wise AND operator, not binary plus. With no knowledge of the relative precedence of the two operators in the following expression the name of the middle operand would cause me to misinterpret the code. Does this change the information content of the expression? Does knowledge of the experimental evidence and the correct operator precedence change the information content (i.e., there is a potential fault in the code because the author may have assumed the incorrect precedence)?

... num_red + all_32_bits_one & sign_bit ...

There is experimental evidence that people use the amount of whitespace appearing between operands and their operators to visually highlight operator precedence

The relative quantities of whitespace used in the following two expressions appear to tell very different stories. Do the two expressions have a different information content?

... x  +  y & z ...
 
...
 
... x + y  &  z ...

The idea of measuring the information content of source code is very enticing. However, an accurate measure requires knowledge of the kind of information a reader is trying to obtain and of information that already exists in their brain.

Another question is the easy with which information can be extracted from code. Something that might be labeled as readability, except that readability has connotations of there being an abundant supply of information to extract.